Author: Paul Nesselroade

Abraham De Moivre and the History of the Normal Curve

(Essay found in Nesselroade & Grimm, 2019; pgs. 135-136) The discovery of the normal curve is usually attributed to Abraham De Moivre (1667 – 1754); being traced to a publication of his from 1733 (De Moivre, 1738; English Translation). He was a friend of people like Edmond Halley (of Halley’s Comet fame) and Sir Isaac Newton and was held in high esteem by the intellectual class of his time. Apparently, Newton occasionally replied to questions with, “Ask Mr. De Moivre, he knows all that better than I do” (Walker, 1934, p. 322). De Moivre’s discovery grew out of his interest

Read More »

Rensis Likert

(Essay found in Nesselroade & Grimm, 2019; pgs. 39 – 40) Use the scale below to respond to the following statement: I enjoy studying statistics. ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree Disagree Neither Disagree nor Agree Agree Strongly Agree If we have ever had to respond to a question in this manner, we can thank the social scientist, Rensis Likert. Likert, born in 1903 in Cheyenne, Wyoming, first began his undergraduate studies in 1922 in civil engineering, but then soon discovered that he preferred to study people instead of inanimate objects (Faculty History Project,

Read More »

Next Steps with Correlations: Scale Development

(Essay found in Nesselroade & Grimm, 2019; pg. 548) A common activity for many academic psychologists is the construction of measuring tools. There are literally hundreds of different psychological traits, tendencies, and abilities that psychologists are interested in measuring; from commonly used concepts like extroversion and neuroticism to less frequently-referenced concepts like humility (e.g., Rowatt et al., 2006) and right-wing authoritarianism (e.g., Mirels & Dean, 2006). The scales used to measure these attributes, however, need to be created. They do not appear out of thin air. Scale development is usually an extensive process.  First, the concept is carefully defined, with

Read More »

Playing with the Numbers: Creating Our Own Sampling Distribution

(Essay found in Nesselroade & Grimm, 2019; pgs. 209- 210) Programs found on the internet allow us actually to see how changing the sample size, mean, and the standard deviation of the population of raw scores change the resulting sampling distribution. Some of the ones recently found online include the StatKey Sampling Distribution for a Proportion program (www.lock5stat.com/StatKey/sampling_1_cat/sampling_1_cat.html), the Rice Virtual Lab in Statistics (onlinestatbook.com/stat_sim/sampling_dist/) and the Rossman/Chance Applet Collection (www.rossmanchance.com/applets/OneSample.html). There are others. A program that is quite flexible, however, is one created by Dr. Patrick Wessa (www.wessa.net/rwasp_samplingdistributionmean.wasp). In this program, we can input the number of replications we

Read More »

With z Scores We Can Compare Apples and Oranges

(Essay found in Nesselroade & Grimm, 2019; pg. 138) Is he taller than he is heavy? This question, at first glance, seems to be nonsensical; like comparing apples with oranges. The reason the question appears to be unanswerable is that height and weight are different variables and measured in different units. How can we say that 6 ft 2 in. is more or less than 145 lb? However, in the world of statistics we can compare the relative position of scores in different distributions by using standardized scores. The z score transformation will convert original scores, from different scales, to

Read More »

The Central Tendency of Likert Scales: The Great Debate

(Essay found in Nesselroade & Grimm, 2019; pgs. 79 – 80) In Chapter 2 readers were introduced to a critical difference between ordinal scales and interval or ratio scales; the nature of the relationship between numerical values. Ordinal scales are a quantitatively organized series of categories, and as such, make no assumptions about the quantitative distance between these categories. Interval and ratio scales hold the intervals constant throughout the measure. In this chapter we learned that the concept of a deviation score is necessary to find a mean. A mean is defined as the point where the deviation scores sum

Read More »

Some Notes on the History of Statistics

(Essay found in Nesselroade & Grimm, 2019; pgs. 46 – 47) Although ancient civilizations like the Egyptians and Chinese used tabulation and other simple statistics to keep track of tax collections, government expenditures, and the availability of soldiers, the modern use of statistics arguably began with the Englishman John Graunt (1620–1674). Graunt tabulated information on death rates in his home town of London and noted that the frequency of certain diseases, suicides, and accidents occurred with remarkable regularity from year-to-year. This realization, by the way, helped to develop the establishment of insurance companies. Graunt also found the occurrence of greater

Read More »

A Strategy for Studying Statistics: Distributed over Mass Practice

(Essay found in Nesselroade & Grimm, 2019; pg. 23) Remember the research on mass versus distributed practice by Baddeley and Longman (1978) that was mentioned earlier in the chapter? The finding that ‘distributed practice is more effective than mass practice’ is actually a very old one, first uncovered by one of the pioneers of psychology, Hermann Ebbinghaus (1885), when conducting his famous studies on memory. It is also one of the more robust findings of psychology; shown to apply to the acquisition of a variety of both neuro-muscular skills like archery (Lashley, 1915) as well as cognitive abilities like face

Read More »

Is the Scientific Method Broken? The Limitations of Science

(Essay found in Nesselroade & Grimm, 2019; pgs. 705 – 706) Throughout this book we have periodically stopped to look more closely at some commonly discussed problems in the world of science; in particular, we have tried to understand better the current reproducibility crisis that is afflicting the social sciences. Let us finish this series by stepping back to look a bit more philosophically at the scientific endeavor as a whole. What can we hope to accomplish with the help of science, and what, if anything, lies on the outside? This has sometimes been referred to as science’s demarcation problem;

Read More »

Is the Scientific Method Broken? The Need to Take Our Own Advice

(Essay found in Nesselroade & Grimm, 2019; pg. 355) Using the 1960 volume of the Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, Cohen (1962) conducted an interesting study. Although the authors of the articles in that volume did not use power analyses, Cohen computed the power of the statistical tests used in each of the studies. According to Cohen’s early guidelines, a small effect size is about 0.20; a medium effect size is around 0.50; and a large effect size is around 0.80. Assuming that the researchers would want to detect a medium treatment-effect size, the average power of the tests

Read More »